That’s Not What a Psychopath Is
Cartoon villains on TV crime shows obscure the huge social and personal costs of psychopathy, as well as the encouraging new science that can help treat it.
By Arielle Baskin-Sommers
We know they are fake, but these famous characters create very real misconceptions about psychopathy. Credit: Alamy (images) Kristen Male (art)
On any given day, millions of Americans curl up to watch their favorite crime shows. Whether it is FBI on CBS, Dexter on Showtime, Mindhunter on Netflix, Killing Eve on BBC, reruns of Law & Order, or any of a myriad of other, similar shows, they draw huge audiences with their vivid portrayals of villains whose behaviors are perplexingly cruel. I’ll confess: I am part of that audience. My students even make fun of how much crime television I watch. I justify some of my TV time as work, providing material for my undergraduate lecture course and for my seminars on the nature of the criminal mind. But I am also captivated by the characters in these dramas, despite—or because of—how unrealistic many of them are.
One of the most common character types on crime TV is the psychopath: the person who commits brutal murders, acts recklessly, and sits stone-cold in front of law enforcement officers. Although the shows are obviously fiction, their plotlines have become familiar cultural touchstones. People watch Agent Hotchner on Criminal Minds label any character who is disturbingly violent as “someone with psychopathy.” They hear Dr. Huang on Law & Order: SVU refer to a youthful offender who hurt a young girl as “an adolescent with psychopathy” who he suggests is incapable of responding to treatment.
Such portrayals leave viewers with the impression that individuals with psychopathy are uncontrollably evil, incorrigible, and unable to feel emotions—a caricature with serious real-life implications. The viewers who devour these shows serve on juries and interpret evidence on the basis of what they think they know. They are lawyers who defend or prosecute individuals on the basis of their frequently inaccurate understanding of psychopathy. They are politicians who enact tough-on-crime legislation, using the public’s fear of and fascination with psychopathy to support draconian measures that serve neither social nor individual well-being.
Over-the-top plots that focus on the atrocities committed by “deranged” offenders are hard to resist because they are specifically designed to target the audience’s deep-rooted fears and assumptions. Crime shows send a message that we “normal” people would never engage in such acts, and that law enforcement exists to protect us from psychopathic predators. These stark, good-versus-evil narratives may have contributed to a system in which individuals with psychopathy are often given harsher sentences, and once incarcerated are placed in solitary confinement for more days, on average, than others in prison. But extensive research, including years of work in my own lab, demonstrate that the sensationalized conceptions of psychopathy used to drive those narratives are counterproductive and just plain wrong.
Iwould like to back up, as I do with my students, and consider what psychopathy really is. It is classified by psychologists as a personality disorder—defined by a combination of charm, shallow emotions, absence of regret or remorse, impulsivity, and criminality—which is why I talk about people with psychopathy rather than the more colloquial psychopaths. About 1 percent of the general population meets the diagnostic criteria of psychopathy, a prevalence roughly twice that of schizophrenia. The exact causes of psychopathy have not been identified, but most scholars conclude that both genetics and environment are contributing factors.
Psychopathy imposes a high cost on individuals and on society as a whole. People with psychopathy commit two to three times as much crime overall as others who engage in antisocial behavior and account for roughly 25 percent of the incarcerated population. They also commit new crimes after being released from incarceration or supervision at a much higher rate than other types of offenders. My colleagues and I have found that people with psychopathy tend to start using substances at an earlier age and try more types of substances compared with others. There is also some evidence that people with psychopathy tend not to respond well to conventional therapeutic strategies.
The rational and compassionate response would be to find ways to reduce the burden of psychopathy. Myths about psychopathy get in the way, however, by painting people with psychopathy as unredeemable and unlike other humans.
Reality is significantly more nuanced and encouraging than the grim media narratives. Contrary to most portrayals, psychopathy is not synonymous with violence. It is true that individuals with psychopathy are more likely to commit violent crime than are individuals without the disorder, but violent behavior is not a requirement for a diagnosis of psychopathy. Some researchers argue that key traits of psychopathy are present in individuals who show no violent behavior but who tend toward impulsive and risky behaviors, take advantage of others, and show little concern for the consequences of their actions. Those traits can be observed in politicians, CEOs, and financiers.
Many crime shows, as well as many mainstream news stories, also associate psychopathy with a lack of emotion, particularly of fear or remorse. Whether a character is standing calmly over a lifeless body or giving the classic “psychopathic stare,” viewers are accustomed to seeing people with psychopathy as almost robotic. The belief that people with psychopathy are emotionless is widespread not only among laypeople but among psychologists as well. There is an element of truth here: Considerable research has found that individuals with psychopathy exhibit a reduced ability to process emotions and to recognize the emotions of others. But my colleagues and I are finding evidence that individuals with psychopathy actually can identify and experience emotions under the right circumstances.
In my lab, we are conducting experiments that reveal a complex relationship between psychopathy and emotions. In one study, we examined the supposed lack of fear of individuals with psychopathy using a simple lab test. We showed a group of participants the letter n and colored boxes on a screen. Seeing a red box meant a participant might get an electric shock; green boxes meant that they would not. The color of the box therefore signaled a threat. In some trials, we asked the participant to tell us the color of the box (forcing them to focus on the threat). In other trials, we asked the participant to tell us the case of the letter (forcing them to focus on the non-threat), although the box was still displayed.
We could see that individuals with psychopathy displayed fear responses based on their physiological and brain reactions when they had to focus on the shock threat. However, they showed a deficit in fear responses when they had to tell us the case of the letter and the box was secondary to that task. Evidently, individuals with psychopathy are capable of experiencing emotion; they just have a blunted emotional response when their attention is directed toward something else. This is an extreme version of the kind of processing we all do. In routine decision making, we rarely focus explicitly on emotion. Rather, we use emotional information as a background detail that informs our decisions. The implication is that individuals with psychopathy have a kind of mental myopia: The emotions are there, but they are ignored if they might interfere with attaining a goal.
Research in my lab and in others has uncovered additional evidence that individuals with psychopathy are capable of experiencing and labeling emotions in the context of observing emotional scenes or faces, the pain of others, and experiences of regret. Here, too, individuals with psychopathy are able to process emotion when focusing on the emotion, but they display deficits when emotion is hard to detect or is secondary to their objective. These findings challenge some long-held conceptions. People with psychopathy can access emotions, apparently; the emotional information just gets stifled by the focus on goals.
Many studies have shown that individuals with psychopathy are great at using information and regulating their behavior if it is directly relevant to their objective; for instance, they can act charming and ignore emotions to con someone. But when information is beyond their immediate focus of attention, they often display impulsive behavior (such as quitting a job without a new one lined up) and egregious decision making (such as seeking publicity for a crime while they are wanted by police). They have difficulty processing emotion, but unlike the common characters on television, they are not inherently cold-blooded. The image of the fearless killer draws on an outdated scientific conception of psychopathy. It now appears that people with psychopathy are awful at balancing emotional awareness with attentiveness to their desires.
One of the most damaging fallacies about psychopathy—in fiction, in the news, and in some of the old scientific literature—is that it is a permanent, unchanging condition. This idea reinforces the compelling good-versus-evil trope, but the latest research tells a quite different story.
Traits of psychopathy naturally decrease over time for many young people, starting in late adolescence into adulthood. Samuel Hawes, a psychologist at Florida International University, and his collaborators tracked more than 1,000 individuals from childhood to adulthood, repeatedly measuring their traits of psychopathy. Although a small group showed persistently high levels of psychopathic traits, more than half of the boys who initially had high levels of those traits trended downward over time and no longer presented with them later in adolescence.
With proper intervention, the prospects for improvement get better. We are finding that youth with traits of psychopathy and adults with psychopathy can change and respond to treatments that are tailored to their needs. Several studies have documented the effectiveness of specific treatments designed to help youth learn to identify and respond to emotions. Parent training that focuses on enhancing the emotional warmth of the caregiver and helping youth identify emotions seems to reduce symptoms and problematic behavior.
In a series of experiments, we have been investigating video games designed to train the brains of individuals with psychopathy by helping them improve the way they integrate information. For example, we show a group of participants a face and instruct them to respond on the basis of the emotion they see and the direction in which the eyes are looking, coaching them to integrate all features of the face. Or we play a game in which we show participants a series of cards and see if they can pick up when we shift the rules, switching which one is a winning or losing card; the subjects are not told when the shift will happen, so they have to learn to pay attention to subtle contextual changes as they go. Our preliminary data show that lab-based tasks like these can change the brains and real-world behavior of individuals with psychopathy.
Such studies open the possibility of reducing the social and personal harm caused by psychopathy. Entrenched misconceptions, however, limit the support for essential research into psychopathy and the development of new treatments, which puts law enforcement officers in danger and leads to broader risks when an untreated individual who has been isolated from others is reintroduced into the community. We need to retire the myth that individuals with psychopathy are fundamentally violent, emotionless, and incapable of change. We should work harder to aid them, so that they can notice more information in their environment and use more of their emotional experience.
Pop culture can help rather than hinder those goals. The behavior of individuals with psychopathy is fascinating—so much so that it does not need to be embellished to make for dramatic plotlines. To their credit, an increasing number of TV shows are incorporating science advisers to improve the accuracy of their content and to help reshape public attitudes. In the early seasons of Criminal Minds, the term psychopathy was egregiously applied to almost any type of heartless offender; in later seasons, after an expert adviser was hired, this term was reserved for characters who more accurately showed the characteristics of the disorder. This is a great step!
Even the most overripe old crime shows can be enlightening if you can also watch them with a critical eye. I encourage fans to join me in yelling at the screen when the common falsehoods about psychopathy pop up. You can become your own forensic psychologist, able to recognize psychopathy as a complex illness that deserves to be treated and understood.
March 23, 2022